|
This blog discusses industry tech things but also sales, marketing, specification, hiring and dare I say "personal development". That is not to say that I'm the one to really develop anyone however on average I read about 20 books a year on a wide variety of topics. From Friedrich Nietzshe and Jordan Peterson to Robert Herjavec from Shark Tank.
From all the reading, I sometimes develop my own ideas or analogies which I think may help others. Some may be common sense but just put in a way to firm up the context to make the info more useful. For this blog I'd like to discuss how to become humble by the 400 year observation of yourself. I'll explain below but before I start let me me mention why its important to be humble. Put simply, if you are humble and know your own weaknesses, only then will you be able to grow and take in new information. So to truly be wise, a person must not only obtain knowledge and life experience but they must understand their limitations. Imagine, if you will, you were able to travel back in time 400 years and found yourself living in those times. Imagine the old things you'd think or superstitions you'd hold. We are a product of our environment and influenced from a baby by our parents, friends, tv, school, and society as a whole. This means that you most likely would not have the same mind had you been borne 400 years ago. What would you tell the 400 year old you? Would you laugh at yourself or somehow view yourself as inferior with delusions and superstitions? Certainly the 400 old self that was subject to living in those times would not only be different but wouldn't they also be closed minded on some topics or possibly think you were odd or too advanced in ways that may not assimilate to that time period? But on many topics, the 400 year old you would be wrong. The old you's inability to see the future or the advancements would mean that its simply beyond their knowledge. Now imagine you could go forward in time 400 years. What would you experience with the 400 future you? Would you be 200 years old? Would you have a mix of AI such as a robotic arm or memory interface? Who knows. But... Would the future you tell you things to blow your mind and also point out ways that you were wrong? Could it be some ideas that you posses now are simply wrong? You cannot see it because in the context of your position, the shared view of how you interpret your religion may simply be different. If you argue that these are static and do not change I'd refer you to look at the 400 year old you and argue differently. What is it that I'm trying to say?? Its simple, I have ideas on things but maybe I'm wrong and maybe you are right. I don't mean on things such as what day it is and I need not give a specific instance. But if... you open your mind to others and listen and try to learn from everyone you meet and give respect to others even if you disagree you'll find harmony and happiness in doing so. It doesn't mean to blindly follow others or even to be a flake. It just means that keeping an open mind while holding your own opinions is not only nice... its wise. The next point to the above is the idea of context. As humans we think in boxes. What that means is that when someone is talking we filter out everything they are saying with the context of our own understanding and put relevance to that topic into a particular box. When two people are debating a topic it is very frequent that from the context that they are both talking they are both correct. We are conditioned as humans to not be able to understand two truths that conflict. So our brains simply shut off all the data that the other person is delivering because it doesn't fit into our box or into our context. If one person is arguing a point as it pertains to say "the local level" and its benefit and another person is arguing a point that pertains to "a societal level" and the argument is based upon whose point is correct they could argue at length. Have you ever watched two people go back and forth over a topic and never agree? The real key is to not speak with the sole intention of telling someone anything but to use your words to first understand. Asking questions such as "so what you're saying is" and repeating their words to give them a chance to correct you. If you do enough digging you'll be sure to understand them fully. Once you fully understand and agree with what they are saying then you can start to explain how you think and why. If the person is resistant you can ask them "Explain what it is that you think I am saying". If the person cannot repeat back to you what you are saying then you may be able to get them on the same page by explaining "Although I understand you do not agree with me, I only want you to know what I was saying". If the person refuses to leave their box and discuss in a civil and respectful way then just remember the old verse: "Give not that which is holy unto the dogs, neither cast ye your pearls before swine, lest they trample them under their feet, and turn again and rend you. I hope that you have found this post useful and if so please leave me a comment below. Humbly yours.... Mark Feeling humble yet? Below are a list of biases that humans possess. Mind blowing... Name Description Ambiguity effect The tendency to avoid options for which missing information makes the probability seem "unknown". Anchoring or focalism The tendency to rely too heavily, or "anchor", on one trait or piece of information when making decisions (usually the first piece of information acquired on that subject) Anthropocentric thinking The tendency to use human analogies as a basis for reasoning about other, less familiar, biological phenomena. Anthropomorphism or personification The tendency to characterize animals, objects, and abstract concepts as possessing human-like traits, emotions, and intentions. Attentional bias The tendency of our perception to be affected by our recurring thoughts. Automation bias The tendency to depend excessively on automated systems which can lead to erroneous automated information overriding correct decisions. Availability heuristic The tendency to overestimate the likelihood of events with greater "availability" in memory, which can be influenced by how recent the memories are or how unusual or emotionally charged they may be. Availability cascade A self-reinforcing process in which a collective belief gains more and more plausibility through its increasing repetition in public discourse (or "repeat something long enough and it will become true"). Backfire effect The reaction to disconfirming evidence by strengthening one's previous beliefs.cf. Continued influence effect. Bandwagon effect The tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink and herd behavior. Base rate fallacy or Base rate neglect The tendency to ignore base rate information (generic, general information) and focus on specific information (information only pertaining to a certain case). Belief bias An effect where someone's evaluation of the logical strength of an argument is biased by the believability of the conclusion. Ben Franklin effect A person who has performed a favor for someone is more likely to do another favor for that person than they would be if they had received a favor from that person. Berkson's paradox The tendency to misinterpret statistical experiments involving conditional probabilities. Bias blind spot The tendency to see oneself as less biased than other people, or to be able to identify more cognitive biases in others than in oneself. Cheerleader effect The tendency for people to appear more attractive in a group than in isolation. Choice-supportive bias The tendency to remember one's choices as better than they actually were. Clustering illusion The tendency to overestimate the importance of small runs, streaks, or clusters in large samples of random data (that is, seeing phantom patterns). Confirmation bias The tendency to search for, interpret, focus on and remember information in a way that confirms one's preconceptions. Congruence bias The tendency to test hypotheses exclusively through direct testing, instead of testing possible alternative hypotheses. Conjunction fallacy The tendency to assume that specific conditions are more probable than general ones. Conservatism (belief revision) The tendency to revise one's belief insufficiently when presented with new evidence. Continued influence effect The tendency to believe previously learned misinformation even after it has been corrected. Misinformation can still influence inferences one generates after a correction has occurred. cf. Backfire effect Contrast effect The enhancement or reduction of a certain stimulus' perception when compared with a recently observed, contrasting object. Courtesy bias The tendency to give an opinion that is more socially correct than one's true opinion, so as to avoid offending anyone. Curse of knowledge When better-informed people find it extremely difficult to think about problems from the perspective of lesser-informed people. Declinism The belief that a society or institution is tending towards decline. Particularly, it is the predisposition to view the past favourably (rosy retrospection) and future negatively. Decoy effect Preferences for either option A or B change in favor of option B when option C is presented, which is similar to option B but in no way better. Denomination effect The tendency to spend more money when it is denominated in small amounts (e.g., coins) rather than large amounts (e.g., bills). Disposition effect The tendency to sell an asset that has accumulated in value and resist selling an asset that has declined in value. Distinction bias The tendency to view two options as more dissimilar when evaluating them simultaneously than when evaluating them separately. Dunning–Kruger effect The tendency for unskilled individuals to overestimate their own ability and the tendency for experts to underestimate their own ability. Duration neglect The neglect of the duration of an episode in determining its value Empathy gap The tendency to underestimate the influence or strength of feelings, in either oneself or others. Endowment effect The tendency for people to demand much more to give up an object than they would be willing to pay to acquire it. Exaggerated expectation Based on the estimates, real-world evidence turns out to be less extreme than our expectations (conditionally inverse of the conservatism bias). Experimenter's or expectation bias The tendency for experimenters to believe, certify, and publish data that agree with their expectations for the outcome of an experiment, and to disbelieve, discard, or downgrade the corresponding weightings for data that appear to conflict with those expectations. Focusing effect The tendency to place too much importance on one aspect of an event. Forer effect or Barnum effect The observation that individuals will give high accuracy ratings to descriptions of their personality that supposedly are tailored specifically for them, but are in fact vague and general enough to apply to a wide range of people. This effect can provide a partial explanation for the widespread acceptance of some beliefs and practices, such as astrology, fortune telling, graphology, and some types of personality tests. Framing effect Drawing different conclusions from the same information, depending on how that information is presented Frequency illusion The illusion in which a word, a name, or other thing that has recently come to one's attention suddenly seems to appear with improbable frequency shortly afterwards (not to be confused with the recency illusion or selection bias). This illusion may explain some examples of the Baader-Meinhof phenomenon, when someone repeatedly notices a newly learned word or phrase shortly after learning it. Functional fixedness Limits a person to using an object only in the way it is traditionally used. Gambler's fallacy The tendency to think that future probabilities are altered by past events, when in reality they are unchanged. The fallacy arises from an erroneous conceptualization of the law of large numbers. For example, "I've flipped heads with this coin five times consecutively, so the chance of tails coming out on the sixth flip is much greater than heads." Hard–easy effect Based on a specific level of task difficulty, the confidence in judgments is too conservative and not extreme enough Hindsight bias Sometimes called the "I-knew-it-all-along" effect, the tendency to see past events as being predictable[48] at the time those events happened. Hostile attribution bias The "hostile attribution bias" is the tendency to interpret others' behaviors as having hostile intent, even when the behavior is ambiguous or benign. Hot-hand fallacy The "hot-hand fallacy" (also known as the "hot hand phenomenon" or "hot hand") is the fallacious belief that a person who has experienced success with a random event has a greater chance of further success in additional attempts. Hyperbolic discounting Discounting is the tendency for people to have a stronger preference for more immediate payoffs relative to later payoffs. Hyperbolic discounting leads to choices that are inconsistent over time – people make choices today that their future selves would prefer not to have made, despite using the same reasoning.Also known as current moment bias, present-bias, and related to Dynamic inconsistency. Identifiable victim effect The tendency to respond more strongly to a single identified person at risk than to a large group of people at risk. Illusion of control The tendency to overestimate one's degree of influence over other external events. Illusion of validity Belief that our judgments are accurate, especially when available information is consistent or inter-correlated. Illusory correlation Inaccurately perceiving a relationship between two unrelated events. Illusory truth effect A tendency to believe that a statement is true if it is easier to process, or if it has been stated multiple times, regardless of its actual veracity. These are specific cases of truthiness. Impact bias The tendency to overestimate the length or the intensity of the impact of future feeling states. Information bias The tendency to seek information even when it cannot affect action. Insensitivity to sample size The tendency to under-expect variation in small samples. Irrational escalation The phenomenon where people justify increased investment in a decision, based on the cumulative prior investment, despite new evidence suggesting that the decision was probably wrong. Also known as the sunk cost fallacy. Law of the instrument An over-reliance on a familiar tool or methods, ignoring or under-valuing alternative approaches. "If all you have is a hammer, everything looks like a nail." Less-is-better effect The tendency to prefer a smaller set to a larger set judged separately, but not jointly. Look-elsewhere effect An apparently statistically significant observation may have actually arisen by chance because of the size of the parameter space to be searched. Loss aversion The disutility of giving up an object is greater than the utility associated with acquiring it. (see also Sunk cost effects and endowment effect). Mere exposure effect The tendency to express undue liking for things merely because of familiarity with them. Money illusion The tendency to concentrate on the nominal value (face value) of money rather than its value in terms of purchasing power. Moral credential effect The tendency of a track record of non-prejudice to increase subsequent prejudice. Negativity bias or Negativity effect Psychological phenomenon by which humans have a greater recall of unpleasant memories compared with positive memories. (see also actor-observer bias, group attribution error, positivity effect, and negativity effect). Neglect of probability The tendency to completely disregard probability when making a decision under uncertainty. Normalcy bias The refusal to plan for, or react to, a disaster which has never happened before. Observer-expectancy effect When a researcher expects a given result and therefore unconsciously manipulates an experiment or misinterprets data in order to find it (see also subject-expectancy effect). Omission bias The tendency to judge harmful actions as worse, or less moral, than equally harmful omissions (inactions). Optimism bias The tendency to be over-optimistic, overestimating favorable and pleasing outcomes (see also wishful thinking, valence effect, positive outcome bias). Ostrich effect Ignoring an obvious (negative) situation. Outcome bias The tendency to judge a decision by its eventual outcome instead of based on the quality of the decision at the time it was made. Overconfidence effect Excessive confidence in one's own answers to questions. For example, for certain types of questions, answers that people rate as "99% certain" turn out to be wrong 40% of the time. Pareidolia A vague and random stimulus (often an image or sound) is perceived as significant, e.g., seeing images of animals or faces in clouds, the man in the moon, and hearing non-existent hidden messages on records played in reverse. Pessimism bias The tendency for some people, especially those suffering from depression, to overestimate the likelihood of negative things happening to them. Planning fallacy The tendency to underestimate task-completion times. Post-purchase rationalization The tendency to persuade oneself through rational argument that a purchase was good value. Pro-innovation bias The tendency to have an excessive optimism towards an invention or innovation's usefulness throughout society, while often failing to identify its limitations and weaknesses. Projection bias The tendency to overestimate how much our future selves share one's current preferences, thoughts and values, thus leading to sub-optimal choices. Pseudocertainty effect The tendency to make risk-averse choices if the expected outcome is positive, but make risk-seeking choices to avoid negative outcomes. Reactance The urge to do the opposite of what someone wants you to do out of a need to resist a perceived attempt to constrain your freedom of choice (see also Reverse psychology). Reactive devaluation Devaluing proposals only because they purportedly originated with an adversary. Recency illusion The illusion that a word or language usage is a recent innovation when it is in fact long-established (see also frequency illusion). Regressive bias A certain state of mind wherein high values and high likelihoods are overestimated while low values and low likelihoods are underestimated. Restraint bias The tendency to overestimate one's ability to show restraint in the face of temptation. Rhyme as reason effect Rhyming statements are perceived as more truthful. A famous example being used in the O.J Simpson trial with the defense's use of the phrase "If the gloves don't fit, then you must acquit." Risk compensation / Peltzman effect The tendency to take greater risks when perceived safety increases. Selective perception The tendency for expectations to affect perception. Semmelweis reflex The tendency to reject new evidence that contradicts a paradigm. Sexual overperception bias / sexual underperception bias The tendency to over-/underestimate sexual interest of another person in oneself. Social comparison bias The tendency, when making decisions, to favour potential candidates who don't compete with one's own particular strengths. Social desirability bias The tendency to over-report socially desirable characteristics or behaviours in oneself and under-report socially undesirable characteristics or behaviours. Status quo bias The tendency to like things to stay relatively the same (see also loss aversion, endowment effect, and system justification). Stereotyping Expecting a member of a group to have certain characteristics without having actual information about that individual. Subadditivity effect The tendency to judge probability of the whole to be less than the probabilities of the parts. Subjective validation Perception that something is true if a subject's belief demands it to be true. Also assigns perceived connections between coincidences. Surrogation Losing sight of the strategic construct that a measure is intended to represent, and subsequently acting as though the measure is the construct of interest. Survivorship bias Concentrating on the people or things that "survived" some process and inadvertently overlooking those that didn't because of their lack of visibility. Time-saving bias Underestimations of the time that could be saved (or lost) when increasing (or decreasing) from a relatively low speed and overestimations of the time that could be saved (or lost) when increasing (or decreasing) from a relatively high speed. Third-person effect Belief that mass communicated media messages have a greater effect on others than on themselves. Triviality / Parkinson's Law of The tendency to give disproportionate weight to trivial issues. Also known as bikeshedding, this bias explains why an organization may avoid specialized or complex subjects, such as the design of a nuclear reactor, and instead focus on something easy to grasp or rewarding to the average participant, such as the design of an adjacent bike shed. Unit bias The tendency to want to finish a given unit of a task or an item. Strong effects on the consumption of food in particular. Weber–Fechner law Difficulty in comparing small differences in large quantities. Well travelled road effect Underestimation of the duration taken to traverse oft-traveled routes and overestimation of the duration taken to traverse less familiar routes. "Women are wonderful" effect A tendency to associate more positive attributes with women than with men. Zero-risk bias Preference for reducing a small risk to zero over a greater reduction in a larger risk. Zero-sum bias A bias whereby a situation is incorrectly perceived to be like a zero-sum game (i.e., one person gains at the expense of another). This blog discusses industry tech things but also sales, marketing, specification, hiring and dare I say "personal development". That is not to say that I'm the one to really develop anyone however on average I read about 20 books a year on a wide variety of topics. From Friedrich Nietzshe and Jordan Peterson to Robert Herjavec from Shark Tank. From all the reading, I sometimes develop my own ideas or analogies which I think may help others. Some may be common sense but just put in a way to firm up the context to make the info more useful. For this blog I'd like to discuss how to become humble by the 400 year observation of yourself. I'll explain below but before I start let me me mention why its important to be humble. Put simply, if you are humble and know your own weaknesses, only then will you be able to grow and take in new information. So to truly be wise, a person must not only obtain knowledge and life experience but they must understand their limitations. Imagine, if you will, you were able to travel back in time 400 years and found yourself living in those times. Imagine the old things you'd think or superstitions you'd hold. We are a product of our environment and influenced from a baby by our parents, friends, tv, school, and society as a whole. This means that you most likely would not have the same mind had you been borne 400 years ago. What would you tell the 400 year old you? Would you laugh at yourself or somehow view yourself as inferior with delusions and superstitions? Certainly the 400 old self that was subject to living in those times would not only be different but wouldn't they also be closed minded on some topics or possibly think you were odd or too advanced in ways that may not assimilate to that time period? But on many topics, the 400 year old you would be wrong. The old you's inability to see the future or the advancements would mean that its simply beyond their knowledge. Now imagine you could go forward in time 400 years. What would you experience with the 400 future you? Would you be 200 years old? Would you have a mix of AI such as a robotic arm or memory interface? Who knows. But... Would the future you tell you things to blow your mind and also point out ways that you were wrong? Could it be some ideas that you posses now are simply wrong? You cannot see it because in the context of your position, the shared view of how you interpret your religion may simply be different. If you argue that these are static and do not change I'd refer you to look at the 400 year old you and argue differently. What is it that I'm trying to say?? Its simple, I have ideas on things but maybe I'm wrong and maybe you are right. I don't mean on things such as what day it is and I need not give a specific instance. But if... you open your mind to others and listen and try to learn from everyone you meet and give respect to others even if you disagree you'll find harmony and happiness in doing so. It doesn't mean to blindly follow others or even to be a flake. It just means that keeping an open mind while holding your own opinions is not only nice... its wise. The next point to the above is the idea of context. As humans we think in boxes. What that means is that when someone is talking we filter out everything they are saying with the context of our own understanding and put relevance to that topic into a particular box. When two people are debating a topic it is very frequent that from the context that they are both talking they are both correct. We are conditioned as humans to not be able to understand two truths that conflict. So our brains simply shut off all the data that the other person is delivering because it doesn't fit into our box or into our context. If one person is arguing a point as it pertains to say "the local level" and its benefit and another person is arguing a point that pertains to "a societal level" and the argument is based upon whose point is correct they could argue at length. Have you ever watched two people go back and forth over a topic and never agree? The real key is to not speak with the sole intention of telling someone anything but to use your words to first understand. Asking questions such as "so what you're saying is" and repeating their words to give them a chance to correct you. If you do enough digging you'll be sure to understand them fully. Once you fully understand and agree with what they are saying then you can start to explain how you think and why. If the person is resistant you can ask them "Explain what it is that you think I am saying". If the person cannot repeat back to you what you are saying then you may be able to get them on the same page by explaining "Although I understand you do not agree with me, I only want you to know what I was saying". If the person refuses to leave their box and discuss in a civil and respectful way then just remember the old verse: "Give not that which is holy unto the dogs, neither cast ye your pearls before swine, lest they trample them under their feet, and turn again and rend you. I hope that you have found this post useful and if so please leave me a comment below. Humbly yours.... Mark Feeling humble yet? Below are a list of biases that humans possess. Mind blowing... Name Description Ambiguity effect The tendency to avoid options for which missing information makes the probability seem "unknown". Anchoring or focalism The tendency to rely too heavily, or "anchor", on one trait or piece of information when making decisions (usually the first piece of information acquired on that subject) Anthropocentric thinking The tendency to use human analogies as a basis for reasoning about other, less familiar, biological phenomena. Anthropomorphism or personification The tendency to characterize animals, objects, and abstract concepts as possessing human-like traits, emotions, and intentions. Attentional bias The tendency of our perception to be affected by our recurring thoughts. Automation bias The tendency to depend excessively on automated systems which can lead to erroneous automated information overriding correct decisions. Availability heuristic The tendency to overestimate the likelihood of events with greater "availability" in memory, which can be influenced by how recent the memories are or how unusual or emotionally charged they may be. Availability cascade A self-reinforcing process in which a collective belief gains more and more plausibility through its increasing repetition in public discourse (or "repeat something long enough and it will become true"). Backfire effect The reaction to disconfirming evidence by strengthening one's previous beliefs.cf. Continued influence effect. Bandwagon effect The tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink and herd behavior. Base rate fallacy or Base rate neglect The tendency to ignore base rate information (generic, general information) and focus on specific information (information only pertaining to a certain case). Belief bias An effect where someone's evaluation of the logical strength of an argument is biased by the believability of the conclusion. Ben Franklin effect A person who has performed a favor for someone is more likely to do another favor for that person than they would be if they had received a favor from that person. Berkson's paradox The tendency to misinterpret statistical experiments involving conditional probabilities. Bias blind spot The tendency to see oneself as less biased than other people, or to be able to identify more cognitive biases in others than in oneself. Cheerleader effect The tendency for people to appear more attractive in a group than in isolation. Choice-supportive bias The tendency to remember one's choices as better than they actually were. Clustering illusion The tendency to overestimate the importance of small runs, streaks, or clusters in large samples of random data (that is, seeing phantom patterns). Confirmation bias The tendency to search for, interpret, focus on and remember information in a way that confirms one's preconceptions. Congruence bias The tendency to test hypotheses exclusively through direct testing, instead of testing possible alternative hypotheses. Conjunction fallacy The tendency to assume that specific conditions are more probable than general ones. Conservatism (belief revision) The tendency to revise one's belief insufficiently when presented with new evidence. Continued influence effect The tendency to believe previously learned misinformation even after it has been corrected. Misinformation can still influence inferences one generates after a correction has occurred. cf. Backfire effect Contrast effect The enhancement or reduction of a certain stimulus' perception when compared with a recently observed, contrasting object. Courtesy bias The tendency to give an opinion that is more socially correct than one's true opinion, so as to avoid offending anyone. Curse of knowledge When better-informed people find it extremely difficult to think about problems from the perspective of lesser-informed people. Declinism The belief that a society or institution is tending towards decline. Particularly, it is the predisposition to view the past favourably (rosy retrospection) and future negatively. Decoy effect Preferences for either option A or B change in favor of option B when option C is presented, which is similar to option B but in no way better. Denomination effect The tendency to spend more money when it is denominated in small amounts (e.g., coins) rather than large amounts (e.g., bills). Disposition effect The tendency to sell an asset that has accumulated in value and resist selling an asset that has declined in value. Distinction bias The tendency to view two options as more dissimilar when evaluating them simultaneously than when evaluating them separately. Dunning–Kruger effect The tendency for unskilled individuals to overestimate their own ability and the tendency for experts to underestimate their own ability. Duration neglect The neglect of the duration of an episode in determining its value Empathy gap The tendency to underestimate the influence or strength of feelings, in either oneself or others. Endowment effect The tendency for people to demand much more to give up an object than they would be willing to pay to acquire it. Exaggerated expectation Based on the estimates, real-world evidence turns out to be less extreme than our expectations (conditionally inverse of the conservatism bias). Experimenter's or expectation bias The tendency for experimenters to believe, certify, and publish data that agree with their expectations for the outcome of an experiment, and to disbelieve, discard, or downgrade the corresponding weightings for data that appear to conflict with those expectations. Focusing effect The tendency to place too much importance on one aspect of an event. Forer effect or Barnum effect The observation that individuals will give high accuracy ratings to descriptions of their personality that supposedly are tailored specifically for them, but are in fact vague and general enough to apply to a wide range of people. This effect can provide a partial explanation for the widespread acceptance of some beliefs and practices, such as astrology, fortune telling, graphology, and some types of personality tests. Framing effect Drawing different conclusions from the same information, depending on how that information is presented Frequency illusion The illusion in which a word, a name, or other thing that has recently come to one's attention suddenly seems to appear with improbable frequency shortly afterwards (not to be confused with the recency illusion or selection bias). This illusion may explain some examples of the Baader-Meinhof phenomenon, when someone repeatedly notices a newly learned word or phrase shortly after learning it. Functional fixedness Limits a person to using an object only in the way it is traditionally used. Gambler's fallacy The tendency to think that future probabilities are altered by past events, when in reality they are unchanged. The fallacy arises from an erroneous conceptualization of the law of large numbers. For example, "I've flipped heads with this coin five times consecutively, so the chance of tails coming out on the sixth flip is much greater than heads." Hard–easy effect Based on a specific level of task difficulty, the confidence in judgments is too conservative and not extreme enough Hindsight bias Sometimes called the "I-knew-it-all-along" effect, the tendency to see past events as being predictable[48] at the time those events happened. Hostile attribution bias The "hostile attribution bias" is the tendency to interpret others' behaviors as having hostile intent, even when the behavior is ambiguous or benign. Hot-hand fallacy The "hot-hand fallacy" (also known as the "hot hand phenomenon" or "hot hand") is the fallacious belief that a person who has experienced success with a random event has a greater chance of further success in additional attempts. Hyperbolic discounting Discounting is the tendency for people to have a stronger preference for more immediate payoffs relative to later payoffs. Hyperbolic discounting leads to choices that are inconsistent over time – people make choices today that their future selves would prefer not to have made, despite using the same reasoning.Also known as current moment bias, present-bias, and related to Dynamic inconsistency. Identifiable victim effect The tendency to respond more strongly to a single identified person at risk than to a large group of people at risk. Illusion of control The tendency to overestimate one's degree of influence over other external events. Illusion of validity Belief that our judgments are accurate, especially when available information is consistent or inter-correlated. Illusory correlation Inaccurately perceiving a relationship between two unrelated events. Illusory truth effect A tendency to believe that a statement is true if it is easier to process, or if it has been stated multiple times, regardless of its actual veracity. These are specific cases of truthiness. Impact bias The tendency to overestimate the length or the intensity of the impact of future feeling states. Information bias The tendency to seek information even when it cannot affect action. Insensitivity to sample size The tendency to under-expect variation in small samples. Irrational escalation The phenomenon where people justify increased investment in a decision, based on the cumulative prior investment, despite new evidence suggesting that the decision was probably wrong. Also known as the sunk cost fallacy. Law of the instrument An over-reliance on a familiar tool or methods, ignoring or under-valuing alternative approaches. "If all you have is a hammer, everything looks like a nail." Less-is-better effect The tendency to prefer a smaller set to a larger set judged separately, but not jointly. Look-elsewhere effect An apparently statistically significant observation may have actually arisen by chance because of the size of the parameter space to be searched. Loss aversion The disutility of giving up an object is greater than the utility associated with acquiring it. (see also Sunk cost effects and endowment effect). Mere exposure effect The tendency to express undue liking for things merely because of familiarity with them. Money illusion The tendency to concentrate on the nominal value (face value) of money rather than its value in terms of purchasing power. Moral credential effect The tendency of a track record of non-prejudice to increase subsequent prejudice. Negativity bias or Negativity effect Psychological phenomenon by which humans have a greater recall of unpleasant memories compared with positive memories. (see also actor-observer bias, group attribution error, positivity effect, and negativity effect). Neglect of probability The tendency to completely disregard probability when making a decision under uncertainty. Normalcy bias The refusal to plan for, or react to, a disaster which has never happened before. Observer-expectancy effect When a researcher expects a given result and therefore unconsciously manipulates an experiment or misinterprets data in order to find it (see also subject-expectancy effect). Omission bias The tendency to judge harmful actions as worse, or less moral, than equally harmful omissions (inactions). Optimism bias The tendency to be over-optimistic, overestimating favorable and pleasing outcomes (see also wishful thinking, valence effect, positive outcome bias). Ostrich effect Ignoring an obvious (negative) situation. Outcome bias The tendency to judge a decision by its eventual outcome instead of based on the quality of the decision at the time it was made. Overconfidence effect Excessive confidence in one's own answers to questions. For example, for certain types of questions, answers that people rate as "99% certain" turn out to be wrong 40% of the time. Pareidolia A vague and random stimulus (often an image or sound) is perceived as significant, e.g., seeing images of animals or faces in clouds, the man in the moon, and hearing non-existent hidden messages on records played in reverse. Pessimism bias The tendency for some people, especially those suffering from depression, to overestimate the likelihood of negative things happening to them. Planning fallacy The tendency to underestimate task-completion times. Post-purchase rationalization The tendency to persuade oneself through rational argument that a purchase was good value. Pro-innovation bias The tendency to have an excessive optimism towards an invention or innovation's usefulness throughout society, while often failing to identify its limitations and weaknesses. Projection bias The tendency to overestimate how much our future selves share one's current preferences, thoughts and values, thus leading to sub-optimal choices. Pseudocertainty effect The tendency to make risk-averse choices if the expected outcome is positive, but make risk-seeking choices to avoid negative outcomes. Reactance The urge to do the opposite of what someone wants you to do out of a need to resist a perceived attempt to constrain your freedom of choice (see also Reverse psychology). Reactive devaluation Devaluing proposals only because they purportedly originated with an adversary. Recency illusion The illusion that a word or language usage is a recent innovation when it is in fact long-established (see also frequency illusion). Regressive bias A certain state of mind wherein high values and high likelihoods are overestimated while low values and low likelihoods are underestimated. Restraint bias The tendency to overestimate one's ability to show restraint in the face of temptation. Rhyme as reason effect Rhyming statements are perceived as more truthful. A famous example being used in the O.J Simpson trial with the defense's use of the phrase "If the gloves don't fit, then you must acquit." Risk compensation / Peltzman effect The tendency to take greater risks when perceived safety increases. Selective perception The tendency for expectations to affect perception. Semmelweis reflex The tendency to reject new evidence that contradicts a paradigm. Sexual overperception bias / sexual underperception bias The tendency to over-/underestimate sexual interest of another person in oneself. Social comparison bias The tendency, when making decisions, to favour potential candidates who don't compete with one's own particular strengths. Social desirability bias The tendency to over-report socially desirable characteristics or behaviours in oneself and under-report socially undesirable characteristics or behaviours. Status quo bias The tendency to like things to stay relatively the same (see also loss aversion, endowment effect, and system justification). Stereotyping Expecting a member of a group to have certain characteristics without having actual information about that individual. Subadditivity effect The tendency to judge probability of the whole to be less than the probabilities of the parts. Subjective validation Perception that something is true if a subject's belief demands it to be true. Also assigns perceived connections between coincidences. Surrogation Losing sight of the strategic construct that a measure is intended to represent, and subsequently acting as though the measure is the construct of interest. Survivorship bias Concentrating on the people or things that "survived" some process and inadvertently overlooking those that didn't because of their lack of visibility. Time-saving bias Underestimations of the time that could be saved (or lost) when increasing (or decreasing) from a relatively low speed and overestimations of the time that could be saved (or lost) when increasing (or decreasing) from a relatively high speed. Third-person effect Belief that mass communicated media messages have a greater effect on others than on themselves. Triviality / Parkinson's Law of The tendency to give disproportionate weight to trivial issues. Also known as bikeshedding, this bias explains why an organization may avoid specialized or complex subjects, such as the design of a nuclear reactor, and instead focus on something easy to grasp or rewarding to the average participant, such as the design of an adjacent bike shed. Unit bias The tendency to want to finish a given unit of a task or an item. Strong effects on the consumption of food in particular. Weber–Fechner law Difficulty in comparing small differences in large quantities. Well travelled road effect Underestimation of the duration taken to traverse oft-traveled routes and overestimation of the duration taken to traverse less familiar routes. "Women are wonderful" effect A tendency to associate more positive attributes with women than with men. Zero-risk bias Preference for reducing a small risk to zero over a greater reduction in a larger risk. Zero-sum bias A bias whereby a situation is incorrectly perceived to be like a zero-sum game (i.e., one person gains at the expense of another).
0 Comments
Your comment will be posted after it is approved.
Leave a Reply. |
Design Life blogColor inspiration from around the world, backstories on design inspiration and informative articles about marketing, membrane pressing and personal development. Author Mark Viers delivers fresh content that will help you and your business grow and thrive. Categories
All
Popular articles Language of Color Breaking membranes? Delamination Tearing on corners Solving wrinkles Solving dog ears Video installing peelstix 3DL for high end kitchens Pompeii design inspiration Archives
August 2024
|